[PDF] ED337359pdf - ERIC




Loading...







[PDF] Introduction to Botany - Moscow State University Botanical Server

7 jui 2021 · Introduction to the Introduction 1 1 Plants, Botany, and Kingdoms Botany is the scientific study of plants and plant-like organisms

BOTANY SIX, AN EXPERIMENTAL COURSE - JSTOR

BOTANY SIX, AN EXPERIMENTAL COURSE W R Hatch the class was instructed to bring it to all meetings Mr Hatch is Professor of Botany in the State

[PDF] 6th Grade Plant Biology MCAS Preparation: an Interactive Qualifying

27 avr 2012 · Through analysis of graded exams, worksheets, and questionnaires, we were able to conclude that our unit was successful in teaching the course 

[PDF] Plant Parts

Tell students that scientists consider plants to have six basic parts Each of these parts has an class will be taking a closer look at plant parts

[PDF] GRADE SHEET of B Sc (Botany) 6th Semester May-2019

A 6 00 8 00 48 00 Yoga in Daily Life OE052 C 3 00 5 00 15 00 22 00 131 00 5 95 2 1631302003 B+ 4 00 7 00 28 00 A 4 00 8 00 32 00

[PDF] Life Science: 6th Grade Mrs Juanita Norris Syllabus for 2022-2023

Life Science: 6th Grade Ecology: Interactions Unit, Ecology: Biotic Factors Unit, Botany Class Participation/Note Taking/Preparedness- 10

[PDF] ED337359pdf - ERIC

comparing the botanical knowledge of nine sixth grade students with the botanical concepts developed in the elementary textbook series,

[PDF] ED337359pdf - ERIC 34886_7ED337359.pdf

DOCUMENT RESUME

ED 337 359

SE 052 249

AUTHOR

Tull, Delena

TITLE Elementary Textbooks versus the Child: Conflicting

Perceptions of Biology.

PUB DATE

Apr 91

NOTE

21p.; Paper ptasented at the Annual Meeting of the

National Association for Research in Science Teaching (Lake Geneva, WI, April 7-10, 1991).

PUB TYPE

Speeches/Conference Papers (150)

EDRS PRICE

MF01/PC01 Plus Postage.

DESCRIPTORS

Academic Achievement; Biology; Cognitive Processes; *Concept Formation; *Elementary School Science;

Ethnic Groups; Ethnography; Grade 6; Intermediate

Grades; Learning Strategies; *Misconceptions; *Plants (Botany); Science Education; Sex Differences; *Teaching Methods; *Textbooks

IDENTIFIERS

Concept Mapping

ABSTRACT

An ethnographic study was conducted with the goal of comparing the botanical knowledge of nine sixth grade students with the botanical concepts developed in the elementary textbook series, Science, by Silver Burdett, 1985. The extent to which the child's conceptual framework resembles that of the scientist and the extent to which the textbook reflects the knowledge base and abilities of the child were determined. Each child's language, meanings, classifications, and interpretations of botanical phenomena were examined. The children participated in a series of six interviews and tasks. The chIldren's names for plants and plant classification schemes were examined using domain analysis, componential analysis, and taxonomic analysis. Concept maps were made from both the children's botanical concepts and those found in the textbook. The concept maps were used to examine the development of each concept. The data revealed that the children have a large body of knowledge about plants and that children's botanical language and meanings appeared to be learned from the lay culture rather than from the textbooks. (28 references) (KR) *********************************************************************** * Reproductions supplied by EDRS are the best that can be made* * from the original document.* **********************************************A************************

Elementary Textbooks

versustheChild:ConflictingPerceptionsofBiology

Delena Tull

Assistant Professor, Biology105 Lewis Science CenterUniversity of Central Arkansas

Conway, AR 72032

(501)

450-3146

U.S. DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATION

Offico ot Educatmn11 Reseatch and Improvern,m

EDUOATIONAL RESOURCES INFORMATIONCENTER (ERIC)

Ct/Ms document nil peen reproduced is

received trOnt the uereon etorgenlet.on onpmalmg

0 Minor chsnpes neve been madetO improve

reproduction outplay

Foil Its of vie* or opm.ons stated m

trnS tint u ment o not necessarily represent&how OE FP posMon ot polity**PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE THIS

MATERIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED ByDel ena Tull

TO THE EDUCATIONAL RESOURCES

INFORMATION CENTER (ERIC)."

A paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the National Association for Research in Science Teaching (NARST), Lake Geneva, Wisconsin,April 7-10, 1991. (Copyright, 1991, Delena Tull) 2

BEST COPY

AVAILABLE

Elementary Textbooks versus

theChild:ConflictingPerceptionsof Biology

Objectives

An ethnographic study was conducted with the goal of comparing thebotanical knowledge of nine sixth grade students with the botanicalconcepts developed inthe elementary textbook series, Science, bySilverBurdett,1985(Tull,1990).Theresearcherwantedtodeterminetheextenttowhichthechild'sconceptual frameworkresembles that of the scientist, and the extent to which the textbookreflects the knowledge base and abilities of the child.

Recent National Assessments of Educational Progress

(Mullis &Jenkins,1988)haveshownanationwidedeclineinstudentachievement in science. This decline has caused greatconcern amongscience educators. Project Synthesis (Harms & Yager, 1981) reportedthat more than 90% of 12,000 teachers surveyed relyon the sciencetextbook for their science curriculum 90-95% of the time. Clearly,"the curriculum is Ow textbook, and the objectivesare those implicitinthetext"(p.20).Thus,inevaluatingthecurrentstatusofchildren's knowledge inscience,the content of science textbooksmust be taken into account.

Theoretical Framework

Research in education has demonstrated that children

come to schoolwith a body of knowledge about the natural world. From the earlystudies of Piaget (1929) to the many studies of the past decade (seeCarey,1985; Helm & Novak, 1983; Osborne & Wittrock,1933)researchershaveexaminedchildren'sexplanationsofnaturalphenomena.Ausubel(Ausubel,Novak, & Hanesian,1978)hasassertedthattheconceptionschildrenbringwith themtotheclassroomstronglyinfluencewhattheysubsequentlylearn.Successful curricula will be based on the foundation of knowledgethe child brings to the classroom.

Hills (1983, p. 268) has suggested that the child's explanations differfrom that of the scientist because thechildisworking within adifferent conceptual framework. Kempton (1981) calls the knowledgeof laymen folk knowledge. Curricula should be designed to assist the

child inbridgingthe gap between folk knowledge andscientific knowledge.

Almost all

recent research on children's explanations of scientificconcepts has dealt with misconceptions about abstract concepts (e.g.,Helm & Novak, 1983). In the fiold of botany, most studies have dealtwith the concept of photosyntheAs (e.g., Barker & Carr, 1989; Smith& Anderson,

1984;Wandersee,1986)orphotosynthesisand

respiration(Murr,1986;Treagust,1988). Only a few researchers

haveexaminedelementaryschoolchildren'sexplanationsforconcrete botanicai phenomena (see Osborne & Freyberg,1985). Alarge gap exists

inour knowledge of what childrenknow aboutbotanical concepts. The current study was designed to help fill

that gap.

Forexample,tounderstandachild'sexplanationofphotosynthesis or reproduction, we also need to find out what the

child knows about leaves and flowers.

Research Design

Using ethnographic interviews (see Spradley,

1979), the researcherexamined the botanical knowledge of ninesixth grade childrenincentral Texas. The children represented a variety of economic andethnic backgrounds. They had a range of sixthgrade achievement

test scores from low to very high (refer to Table 1). Forfull details ofthe research design, refer

toTull,1990. The researcher examinedeach child's language, meanings, classifications, and interpretationsrelated to botanical phenomena. The children participated in a seriesof six individual interviews and tasks. They identified 64 plants fromslides and identified plants in two outdoor field trips. They sorted 74

photographs ofplantsintocategories.Thechildrendeveloped

concept maps of the botanical concepts plant. leaf.flower. agg. Eachchild was asked to explain the functions of leaves and flowers andother plantparts,aswellasthe environmental needs of plants,human uses for plants, the differences between plants and animals,and the differences between living and non-living. The re. earcherdid not introduce any botanical terms, except the word plants, intothe dialog unless the child had used the term. The children's names

forplants and plantclassification schemes were examined using domain analysis,componentialanalysis,and taxonomicanalysis (Spradley,

1979).

An examination was made of the botanical concepts developed in the firstthrough sixth grade textbooks from theSilver Burdett series &dugs., 1985. All statements related

to botany were classified aseither concrete or abstract. All names for plants were classified aseither beingfamiliarorunfamiliartochildrenincentral Texas.Concept maps were made from the botanical statements in

thetext.The concept maps were used to examine the development of each concept. interpretation The Many of the scientific botanical terms found in the teyz were never usedbytheinformantsinthisstudy(e.g.,monocot,dicot, photosynthesis). For other scientific

terms,it was apparent that thechildren had poor understanding of their meanings. For example, atleast seven children were familiar with the term pollen and couldrecognize some examples but no one had any idea of its function. Thechildren in this study used many of the same folk botanical terms

thattheadult layman would use(leaf,flower,petal,seed). The

children sometimes did not use these terms accurately. Although allthe children could recognize accurately most examples of flowers andleaves, three children called leaves flowers on at least one occasion.Three individualscalled somefruitsflowers.Seveninformants

occasionally

calledpetalsleaves. In describing theparts of theflower, only one child used the term stamen and no one used theterm pistil. At least two individuals did not recognize atypical leafforms such as on yuccas and some informants did not consider blades

of grass to be leaves.

Labels such as bud. seed. fruitx. and toast were frequently confused.Several children did not know that seeds are insideberries. Most

knew that when a seed is buried a new plant will grow out of it. Butno one know how a seedis formed and some believed that seeds

appear on trees but that flowers do not.

The children organized plants into categories. Most of the children'snames for plant categories were similar to thoseof the adult layman.The categories used most frequently by the children were ultei,flowers. bushes. plants. vines. LEALL.cactus. leaves. and weeds.Although they did not use a

scientificclassificationscheme, the students' in this study did have a classification scheme that would be recognized bytheadultlayman.Students'meaningsforsome categories (particularly tree,, vine) would be acceptable to a botanist. 4

The children organized

their categorieshierarchically, although the

hierarchies were in many cases poorly defined and fluctuating. Onlysix informants knew that trees are included in the concept pl.Ants.Even theseinformants, however, tendedtouse plants mainly in

reference to herbaceous, non-flowering plants.

Some botanical terms (e.g.,

fruit,herb) have different meanings inthe folk culture and inthe scientific culture. Adult laymen tend touse the label fad/in reference only to fleshy, edible fruits such asapples. Adult laymen also tend to use seed in reference to any typeof dry fruit. The layman's meaning for huh is often strictly culinary.The children's uses of these terms indicated that they were using thefolk cultural meanings rather than the scientific meanings.

AreChildren'sConceptsNaive.Idiosyncratic.BasedonFolk

Knowledge. or Based on Sciquific Knowledge?

Although

eachchildhadcertainidiosyncrasies(forexample,differences in what specimens would be called bushes), there were

manysimilaritiesbetweeninformantsinoverallmeaningsfor categories (particularly prototypes),criteriafor categoryselection,

and other aspects of their botanical knowledge. Some misconceptionsabout abstract concepts (e.g., how plants get "food") were shared bythe majority of the informants. These trends suggest that children'sexplanations for abstract concepts areless idiosyncratic than might

have been assumed previously.

Some botanical concepts clearly have been learned as part of thelanguage and meanings of the folk culture. Names for plants, namesand meanings for plant categories, and the hierarchical relationshipsbetween plant categories all have a basis in folk cultural knowledge.The children's concepts in these areas are not naive or idiosyncratic,

ratherthey have probably beenlearned from parents andpeers. Even though some of the names and meanings for these conceptsmay not be acceptable from the pointof view of thescientific framework, they do have validity inthe framework of thefolk culture. The data from thisstudy lend support toHills'assertion (1983) that the knowledge of the child is based on a commonsense (folk) theoretical framework.

Some explanations

ofbotanical phenomena appeartobebased

mainly on text-taught terminology and it was clear that the studentshad a poor understanding of thatterminology. This might indicate

f; 5

thatvirtuallyallof thechild's knowledgeforthatsubject wasderived from the textbook and that the child did not have any prior

knowledgeinthatarea(e.g.,photosynthesis,reproduction,theenvironmental needs

ofplants,andthe"lifeprocesses"). Whenexplaining some abstract botanical concepts, however, some notionsfrom folk knowledge (e.g, that plants get "food" from the soil) playeda role in the children's interpretations.

Insomecases,thechildrendidhaveexperiencewiththe

phenomenon eventhoughtheyhadnotlearnedthescientificexplanation for that phenomenon. For example, the children in

this

studyknewthatplantsneedsoil,water,andsunlight.Thisknowledge was likely to have come from first hand experience andfolk cultural knowledge as well as from the text. Knowledge of howplants use sunlight, water, and soil, however, is probably not part of

thefolk cultural knowledge. The textbook did not always provideenough information to

fillthe gap between the knowledge availableto the child from the folk culture and the knowledge available to thebotanist from the scientific culture. Thus the child's explanations ofsome abstract botanical concepts tended to be idiosyncratic, based ona mishmash oftext-taught terminology and folk knowledge, withlittle understanding about how the two fit

together.

Inexaminingelementarychildren'sexplanationsforbiologicalphenomena, Lawson (1988) concluded that there was little evidencethat the children had any self-generated theories. The concepts hestudied were mostly abstract concepts (photosynthesis, reproduction,

celltheory). The current data suggestthatexplanationsof some phenomena (especiallytangible phenomena) arederived from the

folk cultural knowledge or are idiosyncratic interpretations based onfolk cultural knowledge. When the phenomena are not observable,the children typically do not have theories and will tend to fall backon text-taught ideas, which are often poorly understood.

Some student misconceptions may have been learned directly from

thetext. For example, the idea that plants rely on carbon dioxidefrom animals was an erroneous concept found

inthetext.This misctinception may also be part of folk cultural knowledge. Insummary,thechildren'sexplanationsforbotanical phenomena camepartlyfromthefolkculture,partlyfromidiosyncratic interpretation, and partly from the text. 7 6

El Lai

are ihe Children's Areas of Szoiga_ inTheirBotanical

Knowledge?

Despitelimitedcoverageof ecologyinthetext,fivechildrenspontaneously provided accurate examples of food chains, and all

five understood that plants are basic to food chains. In the free listing task, the children remembered best the names for useful plants.Although no one hadalarge number of accurate common names forplants,allchildrend;.splayedabilitiesto distinguish between plantspecimensatthegenericlevel. When naming plants,errorsofovergeneralizationrevealedthatthe children recognized similarities betweenspecies belongingtothe same botanical family. Innamingplants,students'wrong answers representededucated guesses ratherthan random responses. Types of errors made in

naming plants (e.g., calling closely related species by the same name)suggested a greater knowledge of plants than the number of errors

alone implies.

Several

students gave richly detailed descriptions of plants. Thesedescriptions were indicative of good observation skills. Most studentsdisplayed good classificationskills. Two children excelled intheir

conceptions of plant categories, generally using category names thatwould be familiar to botanists and selecting specimens that would beacceptable to a botanist. Most of the children's plant categories (e.g.,

trees, flowers)werebasedmainlyonstructural(ratherthan subjective)

criteria.There was general agreement among studentsconcerning what characteristics were valuable in defining categories.Characteristics used to describe plants and to distinguish categoriesoften were the same characteristics that a botanist would choose.

It was of interest to note that the two children who performed well on theplantclassificationtasks performed poorlyintheplant namingtaQ sandscoredlowontheirsixthgradescience

achievemertestscores (refer to Table1). The two students whoperformed the best in the plant naming tasks scored very high onscience achievement test scores but performed rather poorly on the

plant classificationtasks.Learning namesforplantsisarote memorization task whereas classificationof plant categoriesisa process skill.Aretheachievementtestsexaminingonlyrote memorization and overlooking children's abilitiesto perform process 8 7 skills? More research isneeded to examine thistrend ina larger group.

The children in

this study were asked about their play preference(indoor versus outdoor, see Table 1) The two informants (5,6) whoperformed best in the plant naming task preferred outdoor play. Of

thethreeinformants(3,7,9)whoperformedbestonplant

classification,two preferredoutdoorplay.Four informants whopreferred indoor play performed less well on both tasks. These four(1,2,4,8) had moderate tl very high achievement testscores. Theamount of time children spend playing outdoors may be the mostimportant non-school influence on

theirbotanical knowledge and skills. Further research is needed to examine this assertion. The children displayed a preference for naming plants at the genericlevel of abstraction (e.g.,oak) rather thanat more abstractlevels

(e.g.,tree,plant). The generic level appearstobe psychologicallybasic even ina culturein which knowledge of generic common

names islargelylost.Inruralsucieties,itisnotunusualfor individualsto know thenames forseveralhundredplants(see

Brown, 1984). The research suggests that children desire to identifyobjccts at the level of abstraction at which they can easily recognizethem. Children can differentiate between trees, therefore they wantto demonstrate thatabilityby naming thetypes oftrees. Whenchildren do not know the generic names for trees and other plants,

theyfrequentlyusestrategiestoavoidgivingamare abstractresponse (e.g., guessing or making up a name, describing the plantrather than naming it, saying, "I don't know," or giving no response).

Wh 1

Knowledge?I r nAknei, T IB.

Students' misconceptions and lack of knowledge in botany cut across allindividualdifferences.Regardlessofgender,ethnicity,or achievement test scores,allthe childrenirthisstudy had major misconceptions about scientific concepts, both concrete and abstract.

No child correctly named more than 32% of the plants seen in thefield or viewed in slides (using locally accepted common names asthe standard). Five children named fewer than 20% correctly. Clearly,knowledge of common names for plants is no longer a part of the folk

culture of these children. 8 Several students with high achievement test scores had rather poor classificationskills, One child relied on irrelevant attributes such as backgrounds inthephotograph(e.g.,sunsetcolors)for category selection. Some plant categories (e.g.,flower, weeds, plants) were based on subjective criteria (e.g.,pretty or not pretty) or unstable criteria (e.g., presence or absence of flowers).

As detailed above, the children had misconceptions about a numberof concrete botanical concepts (e.g., flowers, leaves,fruits,seeds).Three children did not know that

treesaretypes of plants. Thechildren's lack of knowledge about names for plant parts may be theresult of the lack of guided field experiences with plants. Research is

neededtodemonstratetheeffectivenessoffieldexperiencesin eliminating these misconceptions.

The children had a poor understanding of abstract botanical concepts(photosynthesis, respiration, reproduction, differences between living

andnon-living/plantsandanimals,theneedsofplants,the importance of plants

to humans) despitethefactthat most wererepeated in the text in several grades. At least six children did notknow there

isa relationship between leaves andfood production, and four did not know there isa relationship betweenflowers and reproduction. No one could adequately explain these relationships.

The children's

explanationsfor many abstractbotanicalconceptsrevealed that they had not previously put much thought into

the meanings of the concepts. The children probably had not previously expressedtheir knowledge abouttopicssuchasthedifferences between livingandnon-living,plantsandanimals,and human

dependence on plants. The good news is that these concepts may notbe dogmatically embedded intheir minds, and thus may not be

particularly resistant to change, as long as the scientific explanationcan be seen to make sense in relationshipto the children'sfolk

explanations.

Children'sconcepts,althoughtenacious(i.e.,long

lasting) may not truly be resistant to change. It may simply be thatno one has ever showed the students therelationship of their folk

knowledge toscientific knowledge thereby enabling them to bridgethe gap between the two. Research is needed to demonstrate

the effects of linking folk meanings to scientific meanings for concepts. The Language. Meanings._ and Classifications of the Text,. The textbook review provided art in depth analysis that helps explain 1 0 9

some of thefactorsthat may prevent textbooks from providing ameaningful learning experience for children. 50-79% of the botanyrelated statements in the text for grades 4-6 wern abstract in nature(see Table 2). For example, the concept that "plants make their ownfood" was introduced in grade 3

(theterm photosynthesis wasintroduced in grade 5). Reproduction was explained in grades 4 and

5,at the same time that the names for the parts of a flower were

introduced.

Of the 156 botanical terms (both folk and scientific terms) used inthe elementary textbooks, 46% were not explicitly or adequately

defined or illustrated (e.g., biological clock, buds, bloom, cones, ovary,spores) (refer to Table 3for a grade-by-grade break-down of thedata). Of the terms that were defined, 46% were used in context onlyonce, rot often enough to enable the novice to grasptheir meaning.

Some terms were inaccurately defined (e.g.,

g_v_c_w_un).Berry was never used in the text. Most visual examples of flowers in the textbook were growing on herbs,thusthetext may promote the misconceptionthat flowers

grow only on herbaceousplants. The labelsforthereproductivepaits ware not introduced before grades 4 and 5,although a pilotstudy by the author (Tull,1986) demonstrated that some five yearold children will describe stamens and pistils even when lacking aname for those flower parts.

Although the texl accurately defined seed and fruit, in grades 2 and

6dryfruitsweremistakenlylabeledseeds. The text did notdifferentiate between culinary and botanical meanings for hei u andfruit. By its use of illustration, the text promoted the myth that

all fruits are fleshy and edible.

A high percentage of the plants named in the text do not grow incentral Texas and would, therefore, be unfamiliar to the children inthis study (see Table 4). Using unfamiliar plants as examples mayhave the effect of placing the concrete in the realm of the abstract.

The researcherdocumented 38falseandmisleadingstatements about botany r?.latedconceptsingradesone throughsix.For

example, the second grade text tells the reader that desert plants donot have leaves. The thirdgrade textstatesthat mushrooms are

plants. The sixth grade text states that evergreen trees have reedles(in Texas, many broadleaved trees and shrubs are evergreen). And in

1 1 an aquarium example,the fifthgrade text statesthat "Withoutthe fish, the plants would die.Without theplants.the fishwould die."

This statement. may

have beenresponsiblefor themisconception stated by several children thatplants cannotlive withoutthe carbon dioxide produced by animals.

In a multiple

textbook analysisthat includedthe sameSilver Burdeu series, Meyer,

Crummey, andGreer (1988)found no errorsin the

textbooks. What caused thediscrepancybetween theirresults and those ofthecurrentstudy? Thedivergentresultssuggestthe inadeqracy of sampling fromthe whole text.An in depthreview of a single subject area (inthis case,botany) maybe bettersuited to some aspects of textbookanalysis. In additiontofalsestatements,anumberoftopicswere unnecessarily repeated inseveral grades.For example,the ideathat seeds can grow into new plantswasrepeated ingrades 1, 2,3, and 6.

The needs of

plants werepresented ingrades1,3,4,5, and 6.

Germination is

discussed ingrades 3, 5,and 6. Cactiwereused as examples for explaining waterconservationingrades 4 and6.

Saguaro cactus was

used almostexclusively asthe exampleof cactus in every grade. Thefunctions of rootsand stemsisrepeated in grades 3 and

5. The ideathat plantsmake their ownfood is repeated

in grades 3,

4, 5, and 6.In addition, areview of theseventh grade

textbook, Macmillan LifeScience, 1986,reveals a50-60% concept over'ap with the elementarytexts.

The researcher's

concept mapsof the textrevealed that inthe upper grades concepts were developed using ahigh degreeof complexity.

Three to ten

hierarchical levels perconcept arefound in theconcept maps. Research isneeded tolearn moreabout elementaryschool children's abilitiestoincorporate conceptswith suchcomplexity.

Stayer and

Bay (1989)question whetertextbooks areoverloading

the memory capacities of youngchildren. Intheir own conceptmaps, the children inthis studytypically usedtwo tofivehierarchical levels. Only one child usedfive to sevenlevels for anyconcepts.

Further study

could be doneusing similartechniques toevaluate the hierarchical complexity of thechild's conceptdevelopment.

Haw_ Y 1_

aoes_thtIgsLatagetheapBetween .the.CoDceptwil

Frgmework of the

Child and theScientils2

The third grade

textbook used aclassificationscheme similar to(but 2 1 1 not the same as) that of the layman, stating that seed plants can be

classified as trees, shrubs, herbs, or vines. The fourth grade text useda scientific classification scheme, classifying plants as seed plants ornonseed plants, monocots or dicots. The text did not attempt to show

therelationship and differencesbetweenthesetwo classificationschemes. For example, nu information was given that would assistthe child in understanding how the folk category flowers would fit

within the scientific categories mor ocot and dicot.

The text did not bridge the gap between the child's folk knowledgeand scientific knowledge. The text generally failed

todifferentiate

between botanical and folk meanings for terms (e.g., fruit. herb) andbetween folk and scientific plant classification schemes.

Through false and misleading statements the

text may initiatebut certainly

perpetuatesanumberofstudentmisconceptions.Thelanguage of the text may also promote misconceptions when folk

meaningsconflictwithscientificmeaningsand whenscientific terminology isinadequately defined and illustrated.

The natural abilities of the children (e.g., ability to identify plants atthe genus and family level) were largely ignored by the text. Theelementary textbooks

didnot introducetheconceptsofspecies,genus, and family at all.Rather, the textbooks introduced only themore abstract levelsofthescientificclassification scheme(e.g., monocot, dicot).

Posner (1983)

hasassertedthatstudentswillnot changetheir explanations forscientific phenomena unlesstheyaredissatisfied

withtheirexislingconception.Aspresented.inthetext,theexplanations for scientific phenomena probably do not challenge the

students'existingideasrelatedtoconceptssuchasplantclassification. This may partly explain why the students' ideas havenot changed after exposure to text-taught ideas.

Summary

The data revealed that the children had a large body of knowledgeabout plants. The child's botanical language, meanings, classification

scheme,

andinterpretationsofbotanicalphenomena were moreclosely aligned with that of the adult iayman than with that of thescientist. The children's botanical language and meanings appeared tobe learned from the lay culture rather than from the textbook.

1 3 1 2

The textbooks relied heavily

9nscientific vocabulary and abstractbotanical concepts. The areas of strength for the children were areasneglected by the

textbooks.The textbooksdidnotsucceedin

bridgingthe gap between the knowledge and abilities of the childand those of the scientist. The textbook neglected to guide the child

into anearlyunderstandingoftheconcrete botanical phenomena

(e.g.,the names for the parts of a flower) necessary tothelaterunderstanding of related abstract phenomena (e.g.,

thefunction of those parts in reproduction).

Overemphasis on

academicandabstractconceptsandscientificvocabulary indicate that the textbooks have placed an emphasis onscience as a body of knowledge rather than as a way of thinking. Inits pedagogical emphasis, neglect of concepts related to human usesof plants, ecology and societal issues, and inits lack of inquiry based

experiments,thetextbookclearlydoesnotreflecttherecommendations for science education put forth bythe NationalScience Teachers Association (NSTA, 1982 a & b),the American

Association for the Advancement of Science (AAAS, 1989), and other education organizations. commendations This studyprovidesalargebaseofdatarelatedtochildren's knowledgeofbotanicalconcepts.Thatdatahasimportant ramifications for science education and for those interested inthe study of factors that affect learning.

The research points to a number of areas for improvement of sciencetextbooks. Textbook publishers must reevaluate how concepts aredeveloped in the

text,taking into account the recommendations forscience education developed by the NSTA (1982a & b) and the AAAS(1989). The amount of highly abstract concepts must be reduced,

particularlyin the lower elementary grades, where concrete, hands-on science must take precedence. The amount ofscicntific vocabulary

in textbooks must be reduced and the remaining vocabulary must becarefully developed. New vocabulary should be used in context onseveral occasions and be accompanied with diverse verbal and visualexamples. At the same time, non-essential

repetitionof trivialor highly abstract concepts can be eliminated from the text. Textbook writers may benefit from using concept maps as guides, to 1 4 1 3 assist themindevelopingscientificconcepts,andforusein

comparingconceptdevelopmentfromgradetograde.Falsestatements made in the text can be minimized by the use of expertreviewers.

Teachers may erroneously assume that students share meanings forbotanical terms with the teacher or the text. Teachers should addressthe differences and overlap between folk and scientific terms.

A textbook designed

for nationwide distribution cannot introduce

children tothe names for plants in their region. Teachers can useregional field guides as supplements to text, thus enabling children tolearn the names for local plants and introducing them to the greatdiversity of organisms in

ti,.eplant kingdom. Theresearchindicatesthatelementarychildrenshouldbe introducedtotheconceptsof genus andfamily(andprobably species) before being introduced to the more abstract levels of the taxonomichierarchy.Childrenshouldbegiventheinformation

needed to enable them to understand the relationship between theclassification schemes of the layman and the scientist.

The discoverythatchildrenhave misconceptionsaboutconcrete botanical concepts suggests that hands-on experientialscience has been neglected in the study of botany. The researcher recommends that teachersprovidechildrenwithnumerousexperienceswith living plants of many different types. The researchersuspectsthatstudentignoranceaboutconcrete botanical concepts forms a barriertotheirabilitytounderstand related abstract concepts. Concrete concepts (e.g., names for flowerparts) can be introduced inthe early grades so that these concepts will serveassteppingstonestorelatedabstractconcepts(e.g., reproduction) that can be introduced

inlater grades. For example,waiting to introduce the names for flower parts in grade 4 (as doesthe text) may be too late. It may be as absurd as waiting till then tointroduce the names for familiar objects such as chairs and cars.Field

trips, slides, and photographs can all be used to expose thechild to the diverseformsinwhichflowerscanappear.Bythetime reproduction

isintroduced, the child may have developed a naturalcuriosity about flower function. A longitudinal studyis needed to

determine whether the elimination of misconceptions about concrete botanicalconceptswillresultinbetterunderstanding of related 1 5 abstract concepts. 1 4 Due to the problems with textbooks, this researcher would like to see

less reliance on the textbook in elementary science. In the primarygrades, a textbook may be unnecessary. For example, botany text

couldbe replacedwithchildren'sstoriesaboutplantsandwith

regional field guides toplants. The researcher reiteratesthe NSTA(1982a) recommendation that in grades one through four 50-75% of

scienceinstructionshouldinvolvethedevelopmentofscience process skills. This research demonstrates thatchildren have natural abilities in classification and observation,abilitiesthat need to be encouraged and developed in the elementary years. The outdoors is a naturallaboratoryfor encouraging studentstomanipulate plants, make observations, collectdata,and expresstheirinferences and hypotheses about what they have observed.

Although

thisstudy was conducted with only nineinformants,alarge amount of data was collected from each child. The use of avariety of types of tasks, both structured and unstructured, providedtriangulating evidence insupport of theinternalvalidity of those

data. Despite differences ingender, ethnicity, and achievement test scores between the students in this study,much of their performance was remarkably similar.Furtherstudyisneeded to examine the

extent of specific trends inthe larger population. In comparing thechildren's plant naming strategies with those of children in two other

plant naming studies (Dougherty,

1979,inCalifornia, and Stross,1973, in Mexico) some notable similarities emerge. For a report on

the similarities and differences between these three studies,refer to

Tull, 1990.

t;

References

1 5

American Association

fortheAdvancement ofScience.(1989).Science for All Americans: A Project 2061Report on Literacy Goals inScience, Mathematics, and Technology. Washington,D.C.: AAAS.

Ausubel, D. P., Novak, J.

D., & Hanesian H. (1978). EducationalPsychology: A Cognitive View (2nd ed.). NewYork: Holt, Rinehart, andWinston.

Barker,

M., & Carr,M.(1989).Teachingandlearningaboutphotosynthesis.Part1: An assessment interms of students'priorknowledge. International Journal of ScienceEducation, 11, 49-56.

Brown, C. H. (1984). Language and Living Things.

New Brunswick, NJ:Rutgers University Press.

Carey, S. (1985). Co weptual Change

in Childhood. Cambridge, MA:MIT Press.

Dougheny, J. W. (1979). Learning

names for plants and plants fornames. Anthropological Linguistics, 21, 298-315. Harms, N. C., & Yager, R. E. (Eds.). (1981). What Research Says to theScienceTeacher, Volume3.Washington,D.C.:NationalScienceTeachers Association.

Helm, H.. & Novak, J.

D. (1983, June 20-22). Proceedings of theInternational Seminaron Misconceptions in Science and Mathematics.Ithaca, NY: Department of Education, Cornell University.

Hills, G. (1983, June 20-L2). Misconceptions misconceived?

Usingconceptual change to understandsome of the problems pupils havein learning science. In H. Helm & J. D.Novak (Eds.), Proceedings of theInternational Seminaron Misconceptions in Science and Mathematics(pp.264-275).Ithaca,NY:DepartmentofEducation,CornellUniversity.

Kempton, W. (1981). The Folk Classification of Ceramics. New

York:Academic Press.

Lawson, A. E. (1988). The acquisition of biological knowledge during 7 1 6 childhood: Cognitive conflict or tabula rasa? Journal of Research in

Science Teaching, 25, 185 -1 99.

Meyer, L. A., Crummey, L., & Greer, E. A. (1988). Elementary science textbooks:their contents, text characteristics, and comprehensibility. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 25, 435-463.

Mullis,

I. V. S., & Jenkins, L. B. (1988). The Science Report Card,Elements of Risk and Recovery: Trends and Achievement Based onthe 1986 National Assessment. Princeton, New Jersey: Educational

Testing Service.

Murr, M. H. (1986). An identification of misconceptions in biology, their nature, andtheiraccommodation duringinstruction.Dissertation

Abstracts International, 47, 1270-A.

National Science Teachers Association. (1982a). Science -Technology- Society: Science Education for the 1980s. Position statement. Washington,

D.C.: NSTA.

National Science Teachers Association. (1982b). NSTA Position Statement on School Science Education for the 1980s.Washington, D.C.: NSTA. Osborne, R. J., & Freyberg, P. (1985). Learning in Science: The Implications of Children's Science. Portsmouth, NH: FIeinemann. Osborne, R. J., & Wittrock, M. C. (1983). Learning science: A generative approach. Science Education, 67, 489-5 08. Piaget,J.(1929, reprinted1983). The Child's Conception of the World.

Totowa, NJ: Rowman & Allenheld.

Posner, G. (1983, June 20-22). A model of conceptual change: Presentstatus and prospect. In H. Helm & J. D. Novak (Eds.),Proceedings of

the

InternationalSeminar onMisconceptionsinScienceand

Mathematics(pp.71-75).Ithaca, NY: Department of Education.,

Cornell University.

Smith, E. L., & Anderson, C. W. (1984). Plants as producers: A case studyof elementary science teaching. Journal of Research in Science Teaching,21, 685-698.

Spradley, J.

P. (1979). The EthnographicInterview. New York: Holt, S

Rinehart, and Winston.

1 7 Stayer, J. R., & Bay, M. (1989). Analysis of the conceptual

structure andreasoning demands of elementary sciencetextsatthe primary (K-3)level. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 26, 329-349.

Stross,

B.(1973).Acquisitionofbotanicalterminology byTzeltalchildren. In M. S. Edmonson (Ed.), Meaning in MayanLanguages. Mouton,The Hague.

Treagust, D.

F.(1988). Development and use of diagnosticteststoevaluate students' misconceptions inscience. International Journal ofScience Education, 10, 159-169.

Tull, D. (1986). Folk classification

systems of children in central Texas.Unpublished manuscript. Tull, D. (1990). Elementary Science Students' Conceptions in Biology:

TheirLanguage, Meanings,Classifications, and Interpretationsof ScientificConcepts- An Ethnographic Study. Doctoral di.3sertation, University ofTexas, Austin.

Wandersee, J. H. (1986). Can the history of science help science educatorsanticipatestudents'misconceptions? Journal of ResearchinScienceTeaching, 23, 581-597. 1 9

Table 1

InformantProfiles

Informant Identification Number

Profile

123456789

Male/FemaleFFMMFFMFMEthnicityaANANMAANANANAFANMAIndoor/Outdoor

II0I000II

MAT ScoresbComplete Battery

999329979899508771Science NPC996750999999837259ScienceStaninc965999765

aEthnicity: AN=Anglo-American;AF=African-American;MA=Mexican-AmericanbMAT=MetropolitanAchievement Test 6 cNP=NctionalPercentile

Table 2

Percentage of AbstractBotanical PropositionsinElementary Scisnce Textbooks by SilverBurdett Grade

Ratio%

1

5/3017

213/6121

340/11734459/11750

5149/19676

6135/17079

20

Table 3

Elementary Science Tgluhagkijay_aaygLAArdcu

GradeRatioq.

1

11/1385

213/1681

331/4865

435/5564

527/6045

653/8562

Table 4

Percentage of Unfamiliar Plant Names in

theElementary Science Textbooks by Si lvel Burdett Grade

Ratio%

1

2/1020

21/176

39/9110

429/7240

58/2433

614/5127

2,1
Politique de confidentialité -Privacy policy