[PDF] The Rhythm of Rapport: Interpersonal Synchrony and Social





Previous PDF Next PDF



Limitations of the K:N ratio in the nutrient feed of drip-irrigated

Limites du rapport K:N dans la fertilisation des tomates de serre irriguees au goutte ir goutte comme outil de conduite de la culture. Can. J. Plant Sci.



Rapport 6 Whirlpool K-1502-H-AA K-1502-H-AB Rapport 6

K-1502-H-AA K-1502-H-AB. 37. 34. 44. 38. 45. 37. 21. 33. R. 46. 20. 22. 1-215. 23. 16. 20. 24. 22. RAPPORT™ 5.5' WHIRLPOOLS.



The Rhythm of Rapport: Interpersonal Synchrony and Social

13 Feb 2012 To cite this version: Lynden K. Miles Louise K. Nind



Tourist in the (K)Now: social Rapportage and the performative

Tourist in the (K)Now: social Rapportage and the performative rapport of social media. Karen Savage. School of Fine & Performing Arts University of Lincoln 



Rapport sur les activités de la Commission K

Commission K of URSI “Electromagnetism in Biology and Medicine” was chaired until the New Delhi General. Assembly by Bernard Veyret (France) with Frank 



Rapport dwar il-ka? K/022

RAPPORT DWAR INVESTIGAZZJONI. Ka? Nru: K/022. Ilment: Abbu? ta' poter fil-?atra tal-eks ministru Konrad. Mizzi b?ala konsulent mal-Awtorità tat-Turi?mu ta'.



K:Kontoret for LandedokumentationOSINT-TeamAnalyseprodukter

The report does not include any policy recommendations or analysis. The information in the report does not necessarily reflect the opinion of the Danish 



Rapport dwar il-ka? K/036

RAPPORT DWAR INVESTIGAZZJONI. Ka? Nru: K/036. Ilmenti: Biss ir-rapport jirreferi wkoll g?al business cards tas-Sur Bogdanovic.



UNITED STATES SECURITIES AND EXCHANGE COMMISSION

21 Feb 2019 is not incorporated by reference in this Annual Report on Form 10-K. We make available on or through our website certain reports.



Gambie - 1996 - Rapport de revue du portefeuille

-4(jIk. e' 4::: 1j:l Ijy;!k:r dtjlL4tskll:dt!r 1l. 11:1L. 42j:- JI;)h;El.1lr . - JI. 4k::' ' 1EIk' 1Ir' fy. 4(:) . 1l7'd(:r. 41



[PDF] Agrandissement réduction cours

Propriétés (admises) : Dans un agrandissement ou une réduction de rapport k : • Les longueurs sont multipliées par k et • les mesures des angles sont 



[PDF] homothetiepdf

L'homothétie conserve les alignements et les angles Pour une homothétie de rapport k > 0 les longueurs sont multipliées par k et les aires par k² Exemple: Le 



[PDF] Géométrie Homothéties utilisations détermination et compositions

L'homothétie de centre O et de rapport k est notée H(O;k) Une homothétie porte sur tous les points du plan et pas seulement sur la figure dont on



[PDF] Partie 1 : Agrandissement réduction retournement - maths et tiques

L'agrandissement ou la réduction est défini par le rapport de l'homothétie Par exemple si le rapport est -14 alors l'homothétie agrandie les figures en 



[PDF] HOMOTHÉTIE ET AUTRES TRANSFORMATIONS - maths et tiques

1) Construire l'image du point A par l'homothétie de centre O et de rapport 3 2) Construire l'image du point B par l'homothétie de centre O et de rapport -05



[PDF] Homothétie

La figure 2 est un agrandissement de la figure 1 par l'homothétie de centre O et de rapport k = 2 La figure 2 est une réduction de la figure 1 par l'homothétie



[PDF] Composée de deux homothéties

La composée de deux homothéties est donc une homothétie de rapport kk' si kk' est différent de 1 Page 2 Composée de deux homothéties Résumons La composée 



[PDF] 3ème D IE3 homothétie Sujet 1 2018-2019

a) Construire le centre O et déterminer le rapport k de cette homothétie en justifiant b) Ecrire les égalités faisant intervenir des longueurs de segment 



[PDF] Les Transformations du plan - AlloSchool

Définition1:? est un point du plan et k un nombre réel L'homothétie de centre ? et de rapport k est la transformation qui transforme tout point M du plan 



[PDF] homotheties-cours-de-maths-en-3emepdf

On considère l'homothétie de centre O et de rapport k qui transforme la chaussure verte en la chaussure rouge Page 4 Est-ce un agrandissement ou une réduction 

Propriétés (admises) : Dans un agrandissement ou une réduction de rapport k : • Les longueurs sont multipliées par k et. • les mesures des angles sont 
  • C'est quoi le rapport K ?

    Le rapport de similitude (k) est un rapport entre des longueurs homologues (côtés, périmètres, rayons, circonférences, etc.) de 2 figures semblables.
  • Comment calculer le rapport K ?

    Les aires sont multipliées par 0,52. Les volumes sont multipliés par 0,53. Pour un agrandissement ou une réduction de rapport k, -les longueurs sont multipliées par k, -les aires sont multipliées par k2, -les volumes sont multipliés par k3.
  • Comment calculer le coefficient de réduction K ?

    Pour trouver le coefficient, on divise, par exemple, la plus grande longueur du triangle réduit par la plus grande longueur du triangle initial. 1,8 ÷ 6 = 0,3. Le coefficient de réduction est 0,3.
  • Le rapport d'homothétie se calcule TOUJOURS en divisant la distance entre le centre d'homothétie et l'image d'un sommet par la distance entre le centre d'homothétie et le sommet. (OA'/OA ou OB'/OB ou OC'/OC…).
>G A/, ?H@yyeeNjjR ?iiTb,ff?HXb+B2M+2f?H@yyeeNjjR am#KBii2/ QM Rj 62# kyRk >GBb KmHiB@/Bb+BTHBM`v QT2M ++2bb `+?Bp2 7Q` i?2 /2TQbBi M/ /Bbb2KBMiBQM Q7 b+B@

2MiB}+ `2b2`+? /Q+mK2Mib- r?2i?2` i?2v `2 Tm#@

HBb?2/ Q` MQiX h?2 /Q+mK2Mib Kv +QK2 7`QK

i2+?BM; M/ `2b2`+? BMbiBimiBQMb BM 6`M+2 Q` #`Q/- Q` 7`QK Tm#HB+ Q` T`Bpi2 `2b2`+? +2Mi2`bX /2biBMû2 m /ûT¬i 2i ¨ H /BzmbBQM /2 /Q+mK2Mib b+B2MiB}[m2b /2 MBp2m `2+?2`+?2- Tm#HBûb Qm MQM-

Tm#HB+b Qm T`BpûbX

h?2 _?vi?K Q7 _TTQ`i, AMi2`T2`bQMH avM+?`QMv M/ aQ+BH S2`+2TiBQM hQ +Bi2 i?Bb p2`bBQM, GvM/2M EX JBH2b- GQmBb2 EX LBM/- *X L2BH J+`2X h?2 _?vi?K Q7 _TTQ`i, AMi2`T2`bQMH avM@ +?`QMv M/ aQ+BH S2`+2TiBQMX CQm`MH Q7 1tT2`BK2MiH aQ+BH Sbv+?QHQ;v- kyyN- 98 UjV- TTX838X

RyXRyRefDXD2bTXkyyNXykXyykX ?H@yyeeNjjR

Accepted Manuscript

The Rhythm of Rapport: Interpersonal Synchrony and Social Perception

Lynden K. Miles, Louise K. Nind, C. Neil Macrae

PII: S0022-1031(09)00032-8

DOI:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.02.002

Reference: YJESP 2208

To appear in:Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

Received Date: 19 November 2008

Revised Date: 22 December 2008

Accepted Date: 1 February 2009

Please cite this article as: Miles, L.K., Nind, L.K., Macrae, C.N., The Rhythm of Rapport: Interpersonal Synchrony

and Social Perception, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology (2009), doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.02.002

This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers

we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and

review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process

errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Interpersonal Synchrony

1

Running Head: Interpersonal Synchrony

The Rhythm of Rapport:

Interpersonal Synchrony and Social Perception

Lynden K. Miles, Louise K. Nind and C. Neil Macrae

University of Aberdeen

Word count: 2480

Keywords: Interpersonal synchrony, behavioral coordination, rapport, social perception

Address correspondence to:

Lynden Miles

School of Psychology

University of Aberdeen

King"s College

Aberdeen AB24 2UB

Scotland, UK.

Email: lynden.miles@abdn.ac.uk

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Interpersonal Synchrony

2

Abstract

The temporal coordination of behavior during dyadic interactions is a foundation for effective social

exchange with synchronized actions enhancing perceptions of rapport and interpersonal

connectedness. What has yet to be established, however, are the precise characteristics of behavioral

coordination that give rise to such effects. Informed by a dynamical systems approach, the current investigation considered whether judgments of rapport are influenced by the mode of interpersonal synchrony. In two experiments, participants rated the degree of rapport manifest by a simulated pair of walkers exhibiting various configurations of synchronized strides. The results revealed that the highest levels of rapport were associated with the most stable forms of interpersonal coordination (i.e., in-phase and anti-phase synchrony), regardless of whether coordination between the walkers was conveyed via the presentation of visual (Expt. 1a) or auditory (Expt. 1b) cues. These findings underscore the importance of interpersonal coordination to core aspects of social perception.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Interpersonal Synchrony

3

The Rhythm of Rapport:

Interpersonal Synchrony and Social Perception

“When two things keep happening simultaneously for an extended period of time, the synchrony is probably not an accident." (Strogatz, 2003, p. 2). Performing actions that are similar to, and coordinated with, those of an interaction partner elicits feelings of connectedness and interpersonal rapport (Bernieri, 1988; Bernieri, Davis, Rosenthal, & Knee, 1994; Cappella, 1997; Chartrand & Bargh, 1999; Lakin & Chatrand, 2003; Tickle-Degnen & Rosenthal, 1990). This relationship between coordinated behavior and social perception has been demonstrated in a variety of settings (e.g., parent-child bonding, intimate relationships, teacher-student interactions - see Isabella, Belsky & van Eye, 1989; Julien, Brault, Chartrand & Bégin, 2000; LaFrance, 1979), such that the temporal organization of action is acknowledged to be a critical determinant of successful social exchange. To date, the social implications of behavioral coordination have been documented following both the mimicry of discrete bodily movements (e.g., foot shaking, face touching - see Chartrand & Bargh, 1999; van Baaren, Maddux, Chartrand, de Bouter, & van Knippenberg, 2003) and the synchronization of more continuous sequences of action (e.g., postural movements, facial expressions, gestures - see Bernieri, 1988; Cappella, 1997). Research in this domain has been driven by two primary objectives. First, to establish the emergence of mimicry by aggregating

instances of to-be-imitated behavior (e.g., face touching) during social interactions (e.g., Chartrand

& Bargh, 1999). Second, to examine the relationship between coordinated behavior and perceptions of social connectedness (e.g., Bernieri et al., 1994). While this work has repeatedly demonstrated a positive association between coordination and rapport (see Chartrand, Maddux & Lakin, 2005), it has not yet considered whether variations in the dynamics of synchronized actions modulate the nature of this relationship (Semin & Cacioppo, 2008). That is, does the manner in which behavior is coordinated impact social perception?

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Interpersonal Synchrony

4 Related work in the areas of dynamical systems and human movement science may inform this issue. Dynamical approaches to interpersonal coordination (see Schmidt & Richardson, 2008) consider the nature and degree of synchrony between two (or more) coupled oscillators, such as the recurrent movements of interacting individuals (e.g., the limb movements of two people walking side-by-side). This research has provided valuable evidence for spontaneous and unintentional behavioural entrainment across a range of activities, including walking (e.g., van Ulzen, Lamoth, Daffertshofer, Semin & Beek, 2008; Zivotofsky & Hausdorff, 2007), limb movements (e.g., Issartel, Marin & Cadopi, 2007; Schmidt, Carello & Turvey, 1990), postural sway (e.g., Shockley, Santana & Fowler, 2003), the swinging of handheld pendulums (e.g., Richardson, Marsh & Schmidt, 2005), and the motion of rocking chairs (e.g., Richardson, Marsh, Isenhower, Goodman &

Schmidt, 2007).

Of relevance to the current inquiry, when the movements of individuals (i.e., coupled oscillators) become entrained, two dominant modes of synchrony emerge (Haken, Kelso & Bunz,

1985; Schmidt & Richardson, 2008): in-phase coordination (i.e., a 0° relative phase relationship,

1 whereby the actions of each individual are at equivalent points of the movement cycle); and anti-

phase coordination (i.e., a 180° relative phase relationship, whereby each individual"s actions are at

opposite points of the movement cycle). While the actions of interacting individuals routinely move through the various intermediary stages of the relative phase relationship, over time they typically settle in a state of in-phase or anti-phase coordination (Schmidt et al., 1990). This effect arises

because in-phase and anti-phase coordination reflect the globally stable attractor states for coupled

oscillators (Haken et al., 1985), and indeed interpersonal coordination (Schmidt et al., 1990). Thus, when individuals coordinate or synchronize their actions, two pertinent observations can be made. First, social-cognitive research has revealed that aspects of the interaction notably judgments of social connectedness and rapport are enhanced when behavior is coordinated (e.g., Bernieri, 1988; Chartrand & Bargh, 1999). Second, work from a dynamical systems perspective has 1 In-phase coordination can also be defined as a 360° relative phase relationship.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Interpersonal Synchrony

5 shown that two modes of interpersonal coordination predominate, in-phase and anti-phase synchrony (Haken et al., 1985; Schmidt & Richardson, 2008). The integration of these related yet disparate literatures gives rise to an interesting question. Does the mode of interpersonal coordination impact people"s evaluations of a dyadic interaction? To explore this issue, we assessed the level of rapport associated with a simulated pair of walkers displaying different modes of behavioral synchrony. Coordination mode was manipulated by varying the relative phase relationship between the strides of the walkers and the phase-relevant information was conveyed to participants either visually (Expt. 1a) or auditorily (Expt. 1b).

Method

Participants

Sixty-six participants (31 females) ranging in age from 18-36 years took part in the research in exchange for course credit or £5. Thirty-six participants (19 females) completed Experiment 1a and 30 participants (12 females) completed Experiment 1b.

Stimulus Materials

Experiment 1a (Visual Synchrony): A simple 2-dimensional video animation of a walking figure was created by combining, in sequence, 11 static images of a stick figure at equally spaced

degrees of stride length, ranging from full stride to feet together and back again to full stride. Target

videos were constructed by duplicating the figure so it appeared that two stick characters were walking side-by-side. The phase relationship between the strides of the figures was varied by

shifting the starting point in the animation sequence of one figure relative to the other. A separate

video was created for each of the combinations of the starting positions of the one figure relative to

the other (i.e., ranging from both figures starting at ‘full stride," to one starting at ‘full stride" and

the other at ‘feet together," back to both starting at ‘full stride") resulting in 22 video clips. These

clips represented regularly spaced increments in the relative phase relationship between the two

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Interpersonal Synchrony

6

figures" strides ranging from 0° to 360°. The duration of each clip was 10 seconds and comprised

15 repetitions of the stride sequence for the two figures.

Experiment 1b (Auditory Synchrony): A stereo audio recording was made of the footsteps of an adult male walking in hard-soled shoes on a firm surface at a comfortable pace (i.e., average

inter-stride interval of 650 ms). To create the impression of two individuals walking together (i.e.,

two sets of footsteps), one channel of the recording was time-shifted to create a delay between channels in terms of the onset of each step. The relative phase relationship between the footsteps was manipulated by varying the amount of delay (50 - 600 ms, in 50 ms increments) for each

channel giving a total of 24 audio clips. As for the video stimuli, these clips represented regularly

spaced increments in the relative phase relationship between the two sets of footsteps ranging from

0° to 360°. The clips comprised 19 footsteps in each channel and ranged in duration from 11.4 - 12

seconds.

Procedure

Participants were invited to take part in a study concerning perceptions of walking styles and were tested individually. Following their random assignment to one of the tasks, participants were informed that they would be making ratings of interpersonal rapport from either video (i.e., Expt.

1a) or audio recordings (i.e., Expt. 1b). Participants in Expt. 1a were told that they would be

viewing stick figures that represented the movements of two interacting individuals. Participants in Expt. 1b were instructed that they would be listening to audio recordings of the footsteps of two

individuals walking side-by-side. Following each video or audio clip, participants gave their ratings

of rapport on a 9-point rating scale (anchored, 1 = no rapport at all; 9 = a high level of rapport). The

video/audio clips were presented in a unique random order for each participant. Upon completion of the tasks, participants were debriefed and dismissed.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Interpersonal Synchrony

7

Results

To preserve the structure of the phase manipulations, for the purpose of analysis the data sets were ordered linearly according to the relative phase relationship between walkers"

strides/footsteps. Thus, the first and last levels of the phase manipulation corresponded to in-phase

coordination (i.e., a 0°/360° relative phase relationship). The intermediate levels of the phase

manipulation corresponded to an incremental linear transition of the phase relationship, such that the middle values represented anti-phase coordination (i.e., a 180° relative phase relationship).

Experiment 1a (Video Walkers)

Mean ratings of rapport (see Figure 1, top panel) were subjected to a single factor (Relative Phase Level: 1-22) repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). This yielded a significant effect on participants" ratings [F(21, 735) = 52.76, p < .001, p rep > 0.99, ? p2 = 0.60]. Within- participants contrast tests were employed to examine this effect more closely. These revealed that the pattern of change in ratings of rapport fit a quartic 2 function [F (1, 35) = 209.49, p < 0.0001, p rep > 0.99, ? p2 = 0.86]. Participants perceived the level of interpersonal rapport to be highest when

stride patterns reflected either in-phase or anti-phase coordination, and lowest at the mid-points of

the transitions between in-phase and anti-phase movements (i.e., around 90° and 270° relative phase).

Experiment 1b (Audio Walkers)

As in Expt. 1a, mean ratings of rapport (see Figure 1, bottom panel) were submitted to a single factor (Relative Phase Level: 1-24) repeated measures ANOVA. This yielded a significant effect on participants" ratings [F (23, 667) = 22.10, p < 0.001, p rep > 0.99, ? p2 = 0.43]. Within-

participants contrast tests revealed that the effect of the phase manipulation on ratings of rapport fit

2

Quartic (or biquadratic) functions indicate three points of inflection (i.e., changes of direction) in a curve fitted to the

data as can be seen in the ‘W" shaped curves in both panels of Figure 1.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Interpersonal Synchrony

8 a quartic 2 function [F (1, 29) = 118.53, p < 0.0001, p rep > 0.99, p2 = 0.80]. Replicating the video walkers, interpersonal rapport was highest when footsteps conveyed in-phase or anti-phase coordination. Again, the mid-points of the transitions between in-phase and anti-phase coordination (i.e., 90°and 270° relative phase) were perceived to reflect the lowest levels of rapport.

Discussion

A relationship between behavioral coordination and interpersonal connectedness has been revealed in previous research. For the most part, this work has operationalized coordination very broadly, either in terms of mimicry (e.g., Chartrand & Bargh, 1999; van Baaren et al., 2003) or

synchrony (e.g., Bernieri et al., 1994; Capella, 1997) during dyadic interactions. As a result, little

consideration has been given to the precise nature of the coordination observed and how this may impact the perceived quality of social exchanges. Addressing this issue, we explored whether the characteristics of coordinated action, specifically the mode of interpersonal synchrony modulates third-party judgments of rapport. The results were revealing. Across two experiments, in-phase and

anti-phase synchrony were consistently judged to reflect the highest levels of interpersonal rapport.

Moreover, transitions between in-phase and anti-phase synchrony were accompanied by strikingly consistent gradations in perceived rapport, such that when the relative phase relationship was

arranged linearly (i.e., from 0° to 360°) ratings of rapport revealed a strong curvilinear (i.e., quartic)

pattern (see Figure 1). Importantly, perceiver"s ratings of rapport were lowest when relative phase

approximated 90° and 270° (i.e., the points most distant from in-phase and anti-phase), states which

represent potentially unstable modes of coordination. This relationship was evident when phase- relevant information was presented visually (Expt. 1a) or auditorily (Expt. 1b), thereby demonstrating the generality of the observed effect. This association between the relative phase of coordination and rapport corresponds with the stability of in-phase and anti-phase synchrony and the relative instability of other modes of coordination within natural self-organizing systems (Haken et al., 1985; Strogatz, 2003). It would

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Interpersonal Synchrony

9

appear that stability in the dynamics of an interpersonal coupling creates the appearance of a social

connection or synergy between interacting individuals (Marsh, Richardson, Baron, & Schmidt,

2006) which, in turn, enhances judgments of rapport. That is, stable coordination generates a spatio-

temporal context (i.e., common ground) that can be used to judge the qualitative aspects of social

interaction. In this respect, deviations in critical aspects of the relative phase relationship (e.g.,

remaining coordinated but in unstable modes) are sufficient to disrupt interactional synergy and

undermine perceptions of rapport. What this suggests is that perceivers" naïve theories regarding the

quality of interpersonal interactions appear to correspond almost exactly with the dynamical properties that support and constrain the entrainment of coupled oscillators within real-world physical systems (Haken et al., 1985; Strogatz, 2003). The current findings demonstrate that the manner in which behavior is coordinated is an important determinant of the perception of interpersonal connectedness. However, although the simulated dyads we employed provide a means to isolate synchronous movement from other potentially confounding variables (e.g., sex, emotion, social status), it remains to be seen whether our findings are applicable to the perception of actual, real-world interactions. While previous research has shown that perceptions of psychological attributes based on low-level kinematic information are consistent regardless of whether the movements are represented as videos of actual individuals or as animations of nonhuman entities (e.g., Morewedge, Preston & Wegner, 2007), it is

likely that a host of social and contextual factors (e.g., goals, motives, target characteristics, task

setting) play a significant role in shaping people"s judgments. One useful task for future research

will be to explore this issue. In addition, work should also consider whether qualitative variations in

the synchronicity of behavior influence judgments of rapport when the perceiver is a member of the social unit in question. Given that third-party perceptions of rapport are closely aligned with interactants" reports of the occurrence of rapport (e.g., Bernieri, Gillis, Davis & Grahe, 1996; Cappella, 1997), it is probable that first-person experiences of interpersonal connectedness during an interaction will also be influenced by the nature of behavioral coordination.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Interpersonal Synchrony

10 Of additional theoretical importance is the extent to which the beneficial effects of behavioral synchrony extend to the more cognitive aspects of social interaction. Aside from providing a foundation for enhanced rapport and effective social exchange (Semin, 2007; Wilson & Knoblich, 2005), we suspect that interpersonal synchrony may afford other cognitive advantages to interacting individuals. Wilson and Knoblich (2005), for example, have suggested that coordinated action provides a basis for perceptual emulation, a process through which self-perception can

inform the understanding of other social agents and facilitate observational learning when self/other

actions overlap. Supporting this claim, behavioral coordination has been shown to facilitate action planning and anticipatory control when individuals perform a joint tracking task (Knoblich & Jordan, 2003). Additionally, Wilson (2001) has proposed that coordinating one"s actions with another may facilitate what she termed information flow, the dynamics of knowledge transmission during a social exchange. Through savings in working memory, coordinated behavior may enable attentional resources to be directed toward conspecifics, thereby enhancing the person-perception process. Recent findings provide preliminary support for this viewpoint. In a task designed to tap the incidental acquisition of person knowledge during a social exchange, Macrae, Duffy, Miles and Lawrence (2008) demonstrated that memory for an interaction partner"s appearance and utterances were enhanced following instances of synchronized action. Thus, moving in perfect harmony not

only facilitates the processes vital to the foundations of effective interaction, so too it modulates

core aspects of person perception and understanding.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Interpersonal Synchrony

11

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a grant from The British Academy (SG-48408) awarded to LKM and CNM. LKM was supported by a Research Councils of the UK Academic Fellowship and CNM was supported by a Royal Society-Wolfson Fellowship. Thanks to Brad Miles for recording the footsteps used in Expt. 1b.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Interpersonal Synchrony

12

References

Berneiri, F. J. (1988). Coordinated movement and rapport in teacher-student interactions. Journal of

Nonverbal Behavior, 12, 120-138.

Berneiri, F. J., Davis, J. M., Rosenthal, R., & Knee, C. R. (1994). Interactional synchrony and rapport: Measuring synchrony in displays devoid of sound and facial affect. Personality and

Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 303-311.

Berneiri, F. J., Gillis, J. S., Davis, J. M., & Grahe, J. E. (1996). Dyad rapport and the accuracy of its

judgment across situations: A lens model analysis. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 71, 110-129.

Cappella, J. N. (1997). Behavioral and judged coordination in adult informal social interactions: Vocal and kinesic indicators. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 119-131. Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893-910. Chartrand, T.L., Maddux, W., & Lakin, J. (2005). Beyond the perception-behavior link: The ubiquitous utility and motivational moderators of nonconscious mimicry. In R. Hassin, J. Uleman, & J.A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought II: The new unconscious (pp. 334-361). New

York: Oxford University Press.

Haken, H., Kelso. J. A. S., & Bunz, H. (1985). A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biological Cybernetics, 51, 347-356. Isabella, R. A., Belsky, J., & van Eye, A. (1989). Origins of mother-infant attachment: An examination of interactional synchrony during the infant"s first year. Developmental

Psychology, 25, 12-21.

Issartel, J., Marin, L., & Cadopi, M. (2007). Unintended interpersonal coordination: “Can we march

to the beat of our own drum?" Neuroscience Letters, 411, 174-179.

Julien, D., Brault, M., Chartrand, É., & Bégin, J. (2000). Immediacy behaviors and synchrony in

satisfied and dissatisfied couples. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 32, 84-90.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Interpersonal Synchrony

13 Knoblich, G., & Jordan, J. S. (2003). Action coordination in groups and individuals: Learning anticipatory control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition,

29, 1006-1016.

LaFrance, M. (1979). Nonverbal synchrony and rapport: Analysis by the cross-lag panel technique.

Social Psychology Quarterly, 42, 66-70.

Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14, 334-338. Macrae, C. N., Duffy, O. K., Miles, L. K., & Lawrence, J. (2008). A case of hand waving: Action synchrony and person perception. Cognition, 109, 152-156. Marsh, K. L., Richardson, M. J., Baron, R. M., & Schmidt, R. C. (2006). Contrasting approaches to perceiving and acting with others. Ecological Psychology, 18, 1-38. Morewedge, C. K., Preston, J., & Wegner, D. M. (2007). Timescale bias in the attribution of mind. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 1-11. Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Goodman, J. R. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (2007). Rocking together: Dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination.

Human Movement Science, 26, 867-891.

Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (2005). Effects of visual and verbal interaction on unintentional coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and

Performance, 31, 62-79.

Schmidt, R. C., & O"Brien, B. (1997). Evaluating the dynamics of unintended interpersonal coordination. Ecological Psychology, 9, 189-206. Schmidt, R. C., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (1990). Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 227-247.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Interpersonal Synchrony

14 Schmidt, R. C., & Richardson, M. J. (2008). Dynamics of interpersonal coordination. In A. Fuchs & V. K. Jirsa (Eds.), Coordination: Neural, behavioral and social dynamics (pp. 281-307).

Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

Semin, G. R. (2007). Grounding communication: Synchrony. In A. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (2 nd ed., pp. 630-649). New York:

Guilford Publications.

Semin, G. R. & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). Grounding social cognition: Synchronization, entrainment and coordination. In G. R. Semin & E. R. Smith (Eds.), Embodied grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches (pp. 119-147). New York: Cambridge University

Press.

Shockley, K., Santana, M. V., & Fowler, C. A. (2003). Mutual interpersonal constraints are involved in cooperative conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 326-332. Strogatz, S. (2003). Sync: The emerging science of spontaneous order. New York: Hyperion. Tickle-Dengen, L., & Rosenthal, R. (1990). The nature of rapport and its nonverbal correlates.

Psychological Inquiry, 1, 285-293.

van Baaren, R. B., Maddux, W. W., Chartrand, T. L., De Bouter, C., & Van Knippenberg, A. (2003). It takes two to mimic: Behavioral consequences of self-construals. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology. 84, 1093-1102.

van Ulzen, N. R., Lamoth, C. J. C., Daffertshofer, A., Semin, G. R., & Beek, P. J. (2008). Characteristics of instructed and uninstructed interpersonal coordination while walking side-by- side. Neuroscience Letters, 432, 88-93. Wilson, M. (2001). Perceiving imitatible stimuli: Consequences of isomorphism between input and output. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 543-553. Wilson, M. & Knoblich, G. (2005). The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics.

Psychological Bulletin, 131, 460-473.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Interpersonal Synchrony

15 Zivotofsky, A. Z., & Hausdorff, J. M. (2007). The sensory feedback mechanisms enabling couples to walk synchronously: An initial investigation. Journal of Neuroengineering and

Rehabilitation, 4, 28.

ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

Interpersonal Synchrony

16

0123456789

0123456789

Figure 1:

Mean ratings of rapport as a function of relative phase for E1a (top panel) and E1b (bottom panel).

Error bars indicate 1 standard error of the mean.

Mean Ra

pp ort

Coordination Ratin

g s (1 = None

9 = A lot)

In-phase Anti-phase In-phase (0°) (180°) (360°)

Relative phase relationship between strides

Experiment 1a (Video)

Experiment 1b (Audio)

Mean Ratin

g s of Ra pp ort (1 = No Ra pp ort at All

9 = A Hi

g h Level of Ra pp ort) In-phase Anti-phase In-phase (0°) (180°) (360°)

Relative phase relationship between footsteps

quotesdbs_dbs35.pdfusesText_40
[PDF] figures semblables rapport de similitude exercices

[PDF] fiche métier patissier onisep

[PDF] mémoire sur la satisfaction client

[PDF] cv patissier exemple

[PDF] fiche métier boulanger patissier

[PDF] agent de manutention fiche métier

[PDF] compétence manutentionnaire cv

[PDF] fiche métier secrétaire administrative

[PDF] fiche rome secrétaire

[PDF] manutentionnaire fiche rome

[PDF] code rome agent administratif

[PDF] fiche métier ? remplir

[PDF] rapport de stage audiovisuel pdf

[PDF] ati radeon hd 4600 series caracteristique

[PDF] bilan personnel stage exemple