[PDF] ISLANDS OF RELIABILITY FOR REGULAR MORPHOLOGY





Previous PDF Next PDF





A paradigm is structured in such a way that for each form F of L

rules of Italian are to be considered as independent BSs in Aronoff's sense. The schemata of alternating verb stem roots in Italian conjugation to show ...



Islands of Reliability for Regular Morphology: Evidence from Italian

rules for only a subset of the possible derivations. I assume for present purposes that the grammar of Italian contains rules assigning verbs to a conjugation ...



The Italian verb-noun anthroponymic compounds at the Syntax

১৪ ফেব ২০১২ ... verbal stem” analysis unsufficiently pays attention to the Italian second conjugation whose thematic vowel is -e



Stress on Second Conjugation Infinitives in Italian

structure rules of Italian. But the onset of Class A verbs may well much that went into the second conjugation in Italian became a Class B verb rather ...



MFLE Italian Reference Grammar - Introduction

take the infinitive; remove the ending -are/ere/ire; add the first second and third person endings



The Conjugations of Italian

Whether this is accomplished by a rule of s insertion or by listing the form in the lexicon is immaterial here. (23) valere valso



(Stem and Word) Predictability in Italian verb paradigms: An Entropy (Stem and Word) Predictability in Italian verb paradigms: An Entropy

Table 1: Italian verbs of different conjugations. The first step of the procedure consists in classi- fying verbs according to the patterns of formal al-.



Evaluating Classroom Integration for Card-it: Digital Flashcards for

১৩ জুল ২০২৩ This paper presents Card-it



Stress on Second Conjugation Infinitives in Italian.

%20Manganaro%20&%20Napoli%201987.pdf





Monotonic Paradigmatic Schemata in Italian Verb Inflection

indicative only) each of which can occur with particular inflectional endings and not with oth- ers. This property



Islands of Reliability for Regular Morphology: Evidence from Italian

I assume for present purposes that the grammar of Italian contains rules assigning verbs to a conjugation class if they have been encountered only in forms 



Stress on Second Conjugation Infinitives in Italian

Italian as Class A verbs and verbs with the short conjugation vowel in Wanner (1973) proposes a primary stress rule for Italian whereby stress falls on ...



501 Italian Verbs Barron S 501 Verbs Copy - m.central.edu

Italian Verb Conjugation Card Marcel Danesi 2004-08-06 The rules of grammar applicable to verb usage provides a bilingual list of hundreds more regular ...



ISLANDS OF RELIABILITY FOR REGULAR MORPHOLOGY

I assume for present purposes that the grammar of Italian contains rules assigning verbs to a conjugation class if they have been encountered only in forms 



The Conjugations of Italian

arguments for up to five verb conjugations (see Danesi 1976 and Vin- In fact this rule is needed elsewhere in Italian (e.g.



Palatalisation across the Italian lexicon

Italian has a phonological rule that palatalises velar stops /kg/ into affricates [t?



1 Dissociation in Italian conjugations: A single-route account David

conjugation (and receive their theme vowel by rule)



1 Dissociation in Italian conjugations: A single-route account David

conjugation (and receive their theme vowel by rule)



[PDF] MFLE Italian Reference Grammar - Introduction

take the infinitive; remove the ending -are/ere/ire; add the first second and third person endings singular and plural Note: the -ire verbs have two forms



Italian Verb Conjugation Made Easy: The Essential Guide - FluentU

11 avr 2023 · Italian verb conjugation might be tricky but we'll break it down for you in this straightforward guide Read on to learn how to conjugate 



[PDF] All the basics — clearly explained - my Italian Circle

Nouns pronouns adjecxves and arxcles can be masculine feminine singular or plural and verbs are conjugated according to mood tense person and number



[PDF] Italian Verb Tenses Fully Conjugated Verbs 2nd Edition - Adecco

23 nov 2019 · Thank you enormously much for downloading Italian Verb Tenses Fully conjugations and grammar rules taking a narrower focus to



Italian verb conjugation made easy: the ultimate guide

I'll show you how it works and how to learn rules and endings the smart way: by speaking! Download a PDF with the list of the most common Italian verbs!



Top 24 Most Important Verbs in Italian (Plus PDF Cheat-Sheet & Quiz)

7 nov 2021 · Download your free PDF guide on the top 24 most common Italian verbs Includes conjugations and examples Impariamo insieme!



italian verb tenses chart

Printable Worksheet about Italian Verb Tenses



The Complete Guide to Conjugating Verbs in Italian

24 août 2022 · Learn how to conjugate Italian verbs and why mastering the conjugations of Italian verbs will make you a fluent Italian speaker



[PDF] 501 Italian Verbs

1500 Italian verbs conjugated like the 501 model verbs There are however some exceptions to this rule as you have already seen in this chapter

  • What is the rule for verb conjugation in Italian?

    When you conjugate a regular verb, you take the first part of the infinitive version of the verb and then add on the ending that correlates to the subject, the tense, and the ending of the infinitive version. Depending on the type of verb you're conjugating (-ere, -are or -ire) the endings will be different.
  • How many verb conjugations are there in Italian?

    The conjugation of verbs in Italian is fairly easy. It doesn't take much memorizing, and there are only 12 tenses. The simplest way to conjugate Italian verbs is to identify the verb's infinitive form (the base form of the verb) and then add the appropriate ending to it.24 août 2022
  • Verbs can be used in the active, passive and reflexive forms.

ISLANDS OF RELIABILITY FOR REGULAR MORPHOLOGY:

EVIDENCE FROM ITALIAN

A

DAMALBRIGHT

University of California, Santa Cruz

particularly in the debate between single and dual route models ofmorphology. I present a model ofmorphological learning that posits rules and seeks to infer their productivity by comparing their reliability in different phonological environments. The result of this procedure is a grammar in which general rules exist alongside more specific, but more reliable, generalizations describing subregularities for the same process. I present results from a nonce-probe (WUG) experiment in Italian,inwhichspeakers ratedtheacceptabilityofnovelinfinitivesin variousconjugationclasses. These results indicate that such subregularities are in fact internalized by speakers, even for a

regular morphological process.*1.OVERVIEW OF THE ISSUES. In the course ofthe debate between connectionist

models (Rumelhart & McClelland 1986, MacWhinney & Leinbach 1991) and the dual- mechanism model ofmorphology (Pinker & Prince 1988, 1994), a substantial body of research has developed describing qualitative differences between regular and irregular inflectional processes. 1 This research program has grown to include an impressive number ofdomains, including language acquisition, linguistic productivity, lexical rec- ognition tasks, brain imaging, and language pathology (see Clahsen 1999, Pinker 1999, and Ullman 2001 for overviews). Proponents of the dual-mechanism model argue that differencesin howregularand irregularforms areproducedand processedshowthat the a relatively simple grammar ofwidely applicable rules, while irregular forms are stored in some type ofassociative network outside the grammar. Proponents ofsingle-route models such as connectionist models, in contrast, argue that whatever differences there may be between regulars and irregulars emerge as a result ofthe way that speakers store and generalize over all ofthe words oftheir language-regular and irregular-using a single system. To date, no model-single or dual route-has been implemented that can adequately irregular differences poses interesting challenges for all models of morphological pro- ductivity. Those who believe in a single mechanism must take on the question ofhow a unified model can be used to derive seemingly dualistic behavior. Those who believe in a separate rule component, on the other hand, must address the question ofhow language learners use input data to posit rules, and how rules are evaluated for possible

inclusion in the adult grammar.* Thanks to many people for their helpful criticisms and suggestions, including especially Marco Baroni,

Bruce Hayes, Carson Schu

¨tze, Donca Steriade, audiences at UCLA, the LSA, and the Workshop on the

mistakes are, ofcourse, my own. I am also grateful to the participants ofthe experiment reported here. This

material is based on work supported under a National Science Foundation Graduate Fellowship.1

The termsREGULARand

IRREGULARhave been usedin numerous ways; following the literatureon English past tense formation, I use REGULARto refer to productive, default morphological processes, andIRREGULAR

to refer to unproductive, nondefault processes (even if their formation is straightforwardly describable by

rules). 684

ISLANDS OF RELIABILITY FOR REGULAR MORPHOLOGY 685

In this article, I explore one approach to morphological rule learning, suggested by Pinker and Prince (1988:130-34) and further developed by Albright and Hayes (2002). This approach compares the morphological behavior of different words in order to hypothesize rules and evaluate their effectiveness in explaining existing words. The goal oflearning under this model is to discover the best morphological rules and to provide a quantitative measure oftheir productivity. For reasons that are explained below, however, the very best rules in this system are not necessarily those that one would choose in a traditional linguistic analysis. In particular, in addition to the most general rules (such as 'suffix-dto form the past tense"), the model also finds rules to describe more specific, but more reliable, subregularities for the very same change (such as 'suffix-dafter fricatives"). I use here the term

ISLANDS OF RELIABILITYto refer

to such subgeneralizations about phonological environments in which a morphological process is especially robust. This article presents experimental evidence that such sub- generalizations are in fact internalized by speakers and may coexist in grammars along- side the more general rules. The issue ofsubgeneralizations for regular and irregular processes has implications for various models of morphology. The dual-mechanism model and more traditional approaches to morphology agree that local subgeneralizations about regular processes are absent from the adult grammar. In other words, although irregular processes may be sensitive to subgeneralizations or neighborhood similarity effects, regular processes should be equally applicable to all new inputs, regardless oftheir similarity to existing words (Marcus et al. 1995:196). Prasada and Pinker (1993) present results from a nonce-probe ( WUG) experiment (Berko 1958) showing that novel regulars are not more acceptable when they are similar to existing regulars; subsequent studies on other lan- guages, such as German, Hebrew, Japanese, and Italian, have found similar results (Marcus et al. 1995, Berent et al. 1999, Fujiwara & Ullman 1999, Say & Clahsen 2000). Ifspeakers are in fact insensitive to the distribution ofregulars when applying the regular pattern to novel items, this would be strong evidence that subgeneralizations about regulars are absent from the grammar. But if speakers are sensitive to patterns in the distribution ofregulars and can extend these patterns productively to novel items, then we need a way to represent such subgeneralizations in the linguistic system. Before testing whether existing regulars influence the formation of novel regulars, it is helpful to have explicit quantitative predictions about the nature and degree of their potential influence. Section 2 begins with an outline of a model of morphological learning that makes quantitative predictions about novel formations. This model com- pares the reliability of different morphological processes in different phonological envi- ronments, anduses thisinformation to assigngradient predictedwell-formedness scores to novel regulars and irregulars. 2.T HE MINIMAL GENERALIZATION MODEL. Much ofthe work on regular/irregular dis- to similarity, or lexical neighborhood effects (Bybee & Moder 1983, Prasada & Pinker

1993, Say 1998, Berent et al. 1999), under the assumption that gradient productivity

is best attributed to some sort ofanalogy that makes direct use ofthe lexicon. The notion oflexical neighborhoods has proven useful in modeling other on-line tasks, such as lexical access and phoneme disambiguation (Landauer & Streeter 1973, Coltheart et al. 1977, Luce 1986, Newman et al. 1997). However, 'different bynfeatures" is a rather crude way to measure phonological similarity, so we should be cautious in drawing conclusions from studies that fail to find lexical neighborhood effects under

LANGUAGE, VOLUME 78, NUMBER 4 (2002)686

this definition. Furthermore, even if there is no definition of lexical neighborhoods that adequately describes the productivity ofregulars, we would still not have an argument that regulars are handled by a single, context-free rule; we would merely have an argument that more than memory is involved. The current study employs a model that derives gradient productivity effects using a grammar ofstochastic morphological rules, rather than using the lexicon directly. For present purposes, I take it for granted that there is a morphological grammar that is used to project novel forms, focusing instead on how that grammar could be learned, and what rules it might contain for regulars. I assume here that morphological rules specify the changes that are necessary to project one form (e.g. the past tense) from another form (e.g. the present tense): adding an affix, changing a vowel, and so on. In ical environment (/C-D). For example, the suffixation of the regular past morpheme in English might be expressed as 0/→d/-#.

MINIMAL GENER-

ALIZATIONalgorithm, sketched by Pinker and Prince (1988:130-34) and developed further by Albright and Hayes (2002). The premise of this approach is that language learners explore the space ofpossible phonological environments, looking for those that have especially high reliability for a given change. An environment is said to be an ISLAND OF RELIABILITYwhen its reliability value is higher than the general reliability ofa change. The intuition is that we are looking for statements ofthe form: 'adding -a ´reworks 72% ofthe time in the general lexicon, but ifthe verb happens to end in [min], then it works 96% ofthe time". on the phonological form of the root, such as in the Yidi"ergative suffixes:-duafter consonants and-⎷guafter vowels (Dixon 1977). In these cases, collecting statistics about phonological environments can straightforwardly discover the 'true" environ- ments that condition the alternation; the rules adding-duafter consonants and-⎷gu after vowels will have very high reliability, whereas the 'wrong" rules adding-duafter vowels and-⎷guafter consonants would have low reliability. For other languages, like Italian, where class membership is only somewhat less than arbitrary, even the most comprehensive search could not possibly come up with a clean analysis that predicts the correct conjugation class in all cases based solely on phonological environment. But the minimal generalization approach predicts that ifthere are local subregularities in the data, learners will explore them in an attempt to find the best possible analysis. The minimal generalization learner uses the lexicon to construct a grammar of (largely redundant) symbolic rules, annotated for phonological context and reliability. It takes as its input pairs ofwords that stand in a particular morphological relation-for example, ([present], [past]) or ([1sg], [infinitive]), and learns a set of rules to derive one from the other. 2 It does this by carrying out pairwise comparisons ofall ofthe words in the data set and factors them into their change, their shared material, and the unshared residue. The shared material then forms the description of a phonological environment in which the morphological rule is known to apply. 2 An important issue concerns whether speakers learn to derive forms in multiple directions (e.g. both

[1sg]→[infinitive] and [infinitive]→[1sg]), or whether grammars contain rules for only a subset of the

possible derivations. I assume for present purposes that the grammar of Italian contains rules assigning verbs

to a conjugation class if they have been encountered only in forms that do not reveal this information (such

as the 1sg or a loan word from another language). See Albright 2002a for a specific proposal for how learners

decide which derivations to include in their grammars.

ISLANDS OF RELIABILITY FOR REGULAR MORPHOLOGY 687

O O comparing A: with B: yields C:di r b adad o → are o → are o → areXad?cons ?vc ?dors etc . . .

7- $# !#0 +%( ($-

!"7,%$&,,→#2M7!N*M (N42M&+N*

M&+N4 F. % G + 2M++N* M++N4 F "G- , #+

&) $ %! 2MN→[are]), so the remaining parts ofthe words are compared to locate potentially relevant phonological material in the environment ofthe change. ical change will be limited to the following components. The first component is the CHANGE LOCATION-in this case, the end ofthe word. Immediately adjacent to the change location any number of SHARED SEGMENTS, common to both pairs offorms, may be specified. Continuing to move out from the change location, there may next option- ally be one partially specified segment, defined by a set of

SHARED FEATURES-in this

example, a nondorsal voiced consonant. (For the simulations presented in the next section, the capacity for feature decomposition was not employed because it does not materially affect the results for Italian.) To the left of these partially or fully specified segmentsmayalsobea phonemes. The result ofthis comparison is a morphological rule, in this case changing the 1sg suffix [o] to the infinitive suffix [are] in a very particular phonological environment. This rule is then compared against the data set in order to see how many words fit the description (the SCOPEofthe rule) and how many ofthose words actually involve the same change (its

HITS).

The distributions of scope and hits for different rules can be seen not only as a measure ofeach rule"s popularity but also as a relative measure ofits reliability. We can define RELIABILITYas the ratio ofa rule"s hits to its scope, to provide a 'batting average" for the environment, as in 1. (1) Definition of reliability Number offorms included in the rule"s structural change (?its hits) "$& $ %#"++ "#G "%"# +% 2?its scope) For example, the rule environment in Figure 1 covers twenty-one Italian verbs, in- cluding [ba?dare], [dira?dare], [gra?dare] 'gradate", [gra?dire] 'be agreeable", [zb j a?dire] 'fade", [per?vadere] 'pervade", and sixteen others. However, only six ofthese verbs actually belong to the-areconjugation. Thus, the rule has a reliability of6/21?.286. This ratio is then statistically adjusted, using lower confidence limit statistics (at a confidence level of 75%) following a suggestion of Mikheev (1997). 3

The effect of this

3 The confidence level is a parameter of the model and can range from 50% to 100%; the value of 75%

was chosen in the absence of any independent evidence about the correct value to use. The effect of this

parameter is considered in more detail in Albright 2002b, in which it emerges that varying its value does

not make a significant difference in the results.

LANGUAGE, VOLUME 78, NUMBER 4 (2002)688

adjustment is to downgrade the reliability scores when the sample size is small-that is, when the rule attempts to cover only a few words. The rationale for this is that we are more certain about things that are widely attested. Reliability calculated in this way is basedon typefrequency, andtoken frequencyplaysno role.This accordswith asugges- tion in Bybee 1995 that type frequency is probably the most important factor in deter- mining intuitions about novel words. I return to this issue in §4.3. Albright and Hayes have implemented the minimal generalization algorithm in an automated learner program, which carries out these comparisons iteratively across the entire data set. 4 The result is a list ofthousands ofways to describe the phonological environments surrounding the morphological patterns in the language. The list ofrules constructed by the minimal generalization algorithm includes many that are extremely specific, covering just a few words. But comparing heterogeneous sets of words also gives rise to descriptions that are extremely general, even to the point ofspecifying context-free affixation. The context-free rules also have reliability values associated with them, just like any other rule. For instance, in the Italian simulations described below, the 'null description" which allows the affixation of-a´reto any verb at all (0/→ [are]/-#) has a hits value of1,463 (the number ofactual-a´reverbs in the input set) and a scope of2,022 (the total number ofverbs in the input set). This yields an adjusted reliability value of .717. The context-free reliability value of a class may be referred to as its

GENERAL RELIABILITY.

When the model is asked to derive a novel form, it tries to apply each rule in the grammar to the novel input. Whenever a rule can apply (when its structural description is met), it is used to derive a candidate output. When deriving the infinitive of a novel Italian 1sg [lavεsso], for example, some of rules in the grammar would produce the output [lavεs?sare], while others would produce [la?vεssere], [lavεs?sere], and [lavεs?sire]. Each output is assigned a confidence value, equal to the confidence value ofthe best rule that derives it. Thus, [lavεs?sare] receives the confidence value of the best [o]→[are] rule that can apply to [lavεsso], while [la?vεssere] receives the confi- dence value ofthe best applicable [o]→[ere] rule, and so on. Ifthe novel input falls within an island ofreliability for a particular change, then the output employing that change will receive a high score, because it can be derived by a very reliable rule. Novel inputsoften fallwithin morethan oneisland ofreliability-for example,adding- a ´reafteress, afterss, and after voiceless fricatives in general could all be highly reliable rules; it is assumed that the goodness ofthe output is determined solely by the very best one. Ifthe input does not fall into any island, then the best way a change can apply is by using its general (context-free) reliability. Output scores, like confidence scores, range from 0 to 1. 5 The fact that the rule with the highest reliability gets to apply means that if the system is forced to choose a unique output from among the possible candidate classes, that allows one class to emerge as the globally productive, or regular, class for novel productions. Because there are context-free rules, the regular pattern can apply to novel 4

The approach as it is described here requires pairwise comparisons ofall ofthe words in the input data,

which may seem implausible to some readers because of its inefficiency. It should be noted, however, that

the Java implementation ofthis algorithm runs reasonably quickly-the simulations reported in this paper

with input files of 2,022 verbs run in approximately two minutes each on a 450 megahertz PC. 5 In work on English, we have found that output scores above .6 typically correspond to well-formed

outputs, output scores from roughly .2 to .6 correspond to marginal but conceivable outputs, and output

scores below .2 correspond to ungrammatical outputs.

ISLANDS OF RELIABILITY FOR REGULAR MORPHOLOGY 689

words even when they do not resemble existing words to any degree at all. However, ifthe word happens to contain a phonological environment that is more consistently regular than average, then the regular process can apply with even greater certainty. Note crucially that high reliability values in this model do not stem from high simi- larity to any particular words in the lexicon. Rather, they come from finding a large number ofmorphologically consistent words that can be described by a common struc- tural description, regardless ofhow general or specific that description is. The statisti- cally corrected reliability values are a quantitative expression ofthe strength ofa morphological process within a phonological environment. These are assumed to corre- spond to the well-formedness ratings that humans provide for novel forms. A logical use ofthe minimal generalization algorithm would be to search for the very best (?highest reliability) rules, which would be included in the adult grammar. But because there are islands ofreliability for all changes, including the regular one, in most cases it turns out that the context-free default rule will not be the very best rule in the system. Thus, we have an interesting puzzle: the best rules learned by this model are not the rules that would be chosen by linguists for a traditional linguistic analysis. We therefore must ask what the status of these rules is in the adult language. Ifspeakers are insensitive to islands ofreliability for the regular process, as has been claimed in the dual-mechanism literature, then we need to provide the model with a means ofdiagnosing and discarding them. If, though, we can show that speakers are actually aware ofislands ofreliability for the regular process, this would be evidence that these rules are retained in some fashion. In order to test this question, I conducted a nonce-probe experiment on conjugation class assignment in Italian, to which I now turn. 3.A

N EXPERIMENT TO TEST FOR ISLANDS OF RELIABILITY.

3.1.T HEITALIAN VERB CONJUGATIONS. There are four major verb classes in Italian, illustrated in Table 1. These classes, which I refer to here as the-a´re,-e´re,-ere, and ´reclasses, can be distinguished by what vowel they have in the infinitive and whether their stress falls on the root or on the suffix. 6

SAMPLE

SAMPLE

1sg.SAMPLE

VOWEL STRESS SUFFIX ROOT

presINFINITIVE GLOSS [a]suffix-are rem-?remo re?mare 'row" [e]root-ere frem-?fremo?fremere 'quiver" [e]suffix-ere tem-?temo te?mere 'fear" [i]suffix-ire dorm-?dormo dor?mire 'sleep" T

ABLE1. The four major Italian verb classes.

The relative distribution of the four verb classes, calculated from the 2,022 verb types found in a spoken corpus of half a million words (de Mauro et al. 1993), is given in Table 2. This table shows that the majority ofItalian verbs belong to the default class (-a´re), but two ofthe other classes (-e´reand-ı´re) also contain substantial numbers of verbs. 6

Traditional Italian grammars refer to only three classes: class 1 is-a´re, class 2 is-ere/-e´re, and class

3is-ı´re. I avoid using this classification here because it does not distinguish between-ereand-e´re, which

differ significantly in their frequency and phonological contexts-though for a defense of grouping-ere

and-e´reas a single class, see Napoli & Vogel 1990. For the purposes ofthis article, it is possible to ignore

the handful of verbs that do not fit neatly into one of the four main classes, such asporre'put" andprodurre

'produce".

LANGUAGE, VOLUME 78, NUMBER 4 (2002)690

CLASS NUMBER PERCENTAGE

-a´re1463 72.4% -ere281 13.9% -e

´re42 2.1%

´re197 9.7%

T ABLE2. Relative distribution ofthe Italian verb classes. Verbs must always be inflected in Italian (i.e. they cannot appear as bare stems), but not all inflections unambiguously reveal a verb"s class. 7

The first singular present

indicative ending-o, for instance, is the same for all four classes, as can be seen in the fifth column of Table 1. Consequently, if Italian speakers happened to have heard a nonce verb for the first time in the 1st singular, they would not know what class the verb belongs to. But the infinitive suffix is an unambiguous indicator of the verb"s class. Therefore, if speakers have heard a verb only in ambiguous inflections, they will need to guess in order to produce an infinitival form. My study simulates this situation experimentally by presenting participants with novel verbs in the first singular and then gathering their intuitions about the various possible infinitives. Class 1, or the-a´reclass, is the most productive class, and has been identified as the default in the dual-mechanism literature (Say 1998, Say & Clahsen 2000). Various types ofdata support this claim. First, grammars state that new verbs should belong to the-a´reclass (e.g. Dardano & Trifone 1985). Second, my own experience with native speakers has shown that many (but not all) speakers are also explicitly aware ofthis fact. Finally, previous studies have shown that the-a´reclass is favored by participants in experimental settings, even for items that are similar to existing-ere and-ı´rewords (Orsolini & Marslen-Wilson 1997, Say 1998). 8 As in English, for each of the irregular (nondefault) classes in Italian we can identify at least one robust gang ofphonologically related forms that serve as the prototypical members ofthat class. Davis and Napoli (1994) observe that many-e´reverbs conform to a prosodic template (C [?son] VC), and informal inspection suggests that there may be similar templates for the-ereclass (CVC [?son] C [?voice] ,asin[?tendere] 'stretch", [?prendere] 'take", [?mordere] 'bite") and the-ı´reclass (CVC [?son] C [?voice] ,asin [par?tire] 'leave", [men?tire] 'lie", [skol?pire] 'carve"). Davis and Napoli argue that the template for-e´reverbs has played an active role historically in attracting words into the-e´reclass; similarity studies such as the present one investigate the synchronic force ofsuch templates. 9 7

There are four irregular monosyllabic verb stems that end in a vowel and do not receive an additional

suffixal vowel in the 3sg or singular imperative:sta'stay",fa'do.3

SG/IMPERATIVE",da`'give", andpuo`'be

able to". 8

It should be noted that the-ı´reclass also displays a limited degree ofproductivity, particularly in the

creation ofdeadjectival verbs, and is treated as regular by Orsolini and Marslen-Wilson (1997). This ability

ofmore than one class to apply productively to novel words is not a problem for the model presented here,

in which all inflectional classes are handled by a single mechanism. The issue of the productivity of-ı´reis

discussed further in §4.2. 9

As is perhaps to be expected, none ofthese templates works perfectly, either as a synchronic description

ofconjugation class membership in Italian, or as a historical statement about how verbs have changed classes.

Leaving aside Davis and Napoli"s claims about other Romance languages, even for Italian their template

cannot explain every single Italian verb that has remained in or left the-e´reclass (Maiden 1995, Marotta

1997, Wright 1997). The notion that phonological templates may play a role in defining morphological

classes is nonetheless valid, even ifthese particular templates are not the best way to characterize Italian.

quotesdbs_dbs19.pdfusesText_25
[PDF] italian verb conjugation table

[PDF] italian verbs list with english translation

[PDF] italiano avanzato per stranieri pdf

[PDF] italiano facile

[PDF] italiano per bambini stranieri materiale didattico pdf

[PDF] italiano per bambini stranieri pdf

[PDF] italiano per stranieri materiale didattico pdf

[PDF] italiano per stranieri pdf gratis

[PDF] italien facile conjugaison futur

[PDF] italien facile futur proche

[PDF] italy broadband coverage

[PDF] italy 5g rollout

[PDF] italy civil code english translation

[PDF] italy coronavirus

[PDF] italy coronavirus deaths